Press "Enter" to skip to content

Why We Should Question The Narrative On Masks

For all the science-signaling of its advocates, it’s fairly obvious that mask mania is a psychological affair.

Our recent annual summer drive back to my hometown of Cleveland, Ohio took us through Nashville. While I was waiting to check into our room at the hotel, I noticed a woman entering the hotel lobby. If I had to guess, I would say that we were roughly 15-20 feet away from one another. As I turned towards her, with my mask under my chin, she looked at me and made the following declaration to all who could hear: “In no way will I ever stand near someone who is not wearing a mask. When you are done with this guy, you can come get me outside.” And just last week, while at the grocery store, I walked by a young man in one of the aisles. When he saw that I was not wearing a mask (and he was), an interesting encounter ensued. As we passed by each other, he did a sideways type of bend that enabled him to “move out of the way.” Apparently, the Matrix-style move enabled him to avoid catching SARS-COV-2.

It seems safe to assume that these encounters are not personally unique. What such incidents reveal is the profound psychological character surrounding the nature of masks. At least in America, it is certainly the case that there is a rather broad spectrum on the enforcement of mask mandates. Some businesses and institutions can be less restrictive than others on this enforcement, depending upon the state in which you live. And even within a given state, there tends to be significant variations. In Houston, for example, living in Montgomery County versus Harris County can give two rather different stories surrounding the concern over SARS-COV-2.

Even keeping in mind all these national and local differences, the requirement to wear masks in America is still universally upheld. And one of the primary reasons why this is the case stems from the predominant narrative surrounding the effectiveness of masks themselves. This particular narrative is worth briefly exploring.

The trajectory for the prevailing narrative begins by undermining the veracity of whether masks are as effective as we might suppose. At best, much of the data concerning the effectiveness of masks for slowing the transmission of a respiratory virus like SARS-COV-2 has been rather limited. In fact, according to Marilyn Singleton, M.D., J.D., the recommendation to universalize mask-wearing “was published without a single scientific paper or other information provided to support that cloth masks actually provide any respiratory protection.” Since the truth claim about masks plays little substantive role in this regard, then the structure of the narrative as we experience it can now become a bit clearer.

Perhaps more than anything else, it is the psychological character of wearing a mask that is significant. In other words, the most compelling effect of wearing a mask is the psychological satisfaction and feeling of safety that it provides for the individual. Arthur Allen wrote precisely on this point when contrasting the narratives surrounding the 1968 Asian Flu and that of 2009 Swine Flu. For Allen:

How we categorize what happens around us can profoundly affect our perceptions of risk. In 1968, Americans had not been exposed to a steady stream of disturbing news about a bird flu virus in Asia. Pandemic was not a household word associated with terror and globalism, the way it is now. Although many noticed that 1968 was a bad flu year, most of us lacked a doom-laden category in which to place that information. We went about our business, free of excessive virus fright. We’ve all heard, now, about how fearsome pandemics can be, and thanks to the remarkable advances of molecular biology and computing, we have an easier time identifying them. But it turns out that putting a name to something is not only a way of taming it. It can also be a way of spreading needless fear.

With Allen’s reasoning in mind, it seems increasingly evident that the driving narrative behind mask mandates is one rooted in fear. And this helps to better explain why the psychological character of wearing a mask is so convincing and effective. We can feel comforted knowing that we are globally united to help “flatten the curve,” or simply to “mask up.”

Original Content Link

One Comment

  1. Jim McDaniel Jim McDaniel October 10, 2020

    > > cdc states covid19 tests don’t exist – cited in German lawsuit – ((there are two links: The video. and the referenced CDC document found hidden on the FDA website))
    > > https://www.brighteon.com/cb101719-5bcf-4447-9298-887b8c2afbb9
    > >
    > > Starting at appro 24 mins in this video, the speaker states:
    > >
    > > ‘pcr tests are not approved for diagnostic purposes and are incapable
    > > of diagnosing any disease.
    > >
    > > Page 38 of one of the cdc’s publications on the coronavirus:
    ((https://www.fda.gov/media/134922/download))> > ((page39 states that there is no isolated virus))
    > > July 13, 2020
    > >
    > > . Detection of viral RNA may not indicate the presence of infectious
    > > virus or that 2019ncov is the causative agent for clinical symptoms.
    > >
    > > . The performance of this test has not been established for
    > > monitoring treatment of 2019ncov
    > > infection.
    > >
    > > . This test cannot rule out diseases caused by other bacterial or
    > > viral pathogens.
    > >
    > > The drusden pcr texts and the WHO pcr tests are set at 45 cycles and
    > > anything over 35 cycles as reported by the NY times and others,
    > > considered completely unreliable.

Comments are closed.

Breaking News: